Skip to main content

Effectiveness of internet-supported cognitive behavioral and chronobiological interventions and effect moderation by insomnia subtype: study protocol of a randomized controlled trial

Abstract

Background

DSM-V criteria for insomnia disorder are met by 6 to 10 % of the adult population. Insomnia has severe consequences for health and society. One of the most common treatments provided by primary caregivers is pharmacological treatment, which is far from optimal and has not been recommended since a 2005 consensus report of the National Institutes of Health. The recommended treatment is Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Insomnia. Effectiveness, however, is still limited. Only a few studies have evaluated the effectiveness of chronobiological treatments, including the timed application of bright light, physical activity and body warming. Another opportunity for optimization of treatment is based on the idea that the people suffering from insomnia most likely represent a heterogeneous mix of subtypes, with different underlying causes and expected treatment responses.

The present study aims to evaluate the possibility for optimizing insomnia treatment along the principles of personalized and stratified medicine. It evaluates the following:

  1. 1.

    The relative effectiveness of internet-supported cognitive behavioral therapy, bright light, physical activity and body warming;

  2. 2.

    Whether the effectiveness of internet-supported cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia can be augmented by simultaneous or prior application of bright light, physical activity and body warming; and

  3. 3.

    Whether the effectiveness of the interventions and their combination are moderated by the insomnia subtype.

Methods/Design

In a repeated measures, placebo-controlled, randomized clinical trial that included 160 people diagnosed with insomnia disorder, we are evaluating the relative effectiveness of 4 intervention weeks. Primary outcome is subjective sleep efficiency, quantified using a sleep diary. Secondary outcomes include other complaints of sleep and daytime functioning, health-related cost estimates and actigraphic objective sleep estimates. Compliance will be monitored both subjectively and objectively using activity, light and temperature sensors. Insomnia subtypes will be assessed using questionnaires. Mixed effect models will be used to evaluate intervention effects and moderation by insomnia subtype ratings.

Discussion

The current study addresses multiple opportunities to optimize and personalize treatment of insomnia disorder.

Trial registration

Netherlands National Trial Register NTR4010, 4 June 2013.

Peer Review reports

Background

Dissatisfaction with sleep is reported by about 25 % of the adult Western population [1]. The prevalence is even higher in older people and twice as high in women as in men [2, 3]. When complaints about difficulties falling asleep, maintaining sleep, early morning awakenings or nonrestorative sleep are accompanied by physical or cognitive daytime complaints, and last for at least 3 months, occurring at least three times a week, without being secondary to environmental factors or comorbidities that prohibit sleep, a person suffers from insomnia disorder, according to the DSM-V [4]. These criteria are met by 6 to 10 % of adults [2]. Daytime complaints of people with insomnia concern cognitive functioning [5], depressed mood [6] and fatigue [6]. Additionally, people reporting insomnia or low sleep quality have higher risks of depression [7], metabolic diseases and cardiovascular problems [8]. These daytime consequences and comorbidities lead to a reduction in work productivity, to increased sick leave and healthcare consumption and consequently to high economic costs. In summary, insomnia has severe consequences for health and society [9], underscoring the importance of optimizing interventions to promote better sleep. To do so, the regulation of sleep needs to be considered.

Circadian rhythm and sleep

Virtually all behavioral and physiological processes show a 24-h rhythm even in the absence of environmental time cues. This circadian (from circa = about, and dies = day) rhythm is, for example, present in sleep and wakefulness, hormone secretion, alertness, body temperature, metabolism, perception and cognition. Endogenous clock mechanisms that promote the expression of 24-h rhythms are located in all human cells [10] and are synchronized by the central clock of the brain, the hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN). The SCN is considered the major clock because only SCN tissue is able to maintain a circadian rhythm in the absence of external input [10]. Under normal circumstances, several exogenous cues, or zeitgebers, entrain the SCN. Light is the strongest zeitgeber [11]. The retinohypothalamic tract (RHT) is one of the three major input pathways of the SCN, providing it with information from photoreceptor cells in the retina. Secondary zeitgebers include, for example, the temperature of the environment, body core and skin; metabolism; and the rest-activity cycle. When the circadian rhythms of the body are synchronized with the zeitgebers, the rhythm is optimally entrained. A type of insomnia that is characterized mostly by complaints about initiating sleep may be due to a misalignment of the circadian rhythm in sleep-promoting physiology and the sleep window the patient tries to adhere to, typically between 23:00 and 7:00 [12, 13].

Homeostasis of sleep

In addition to the circadian regulation of sleep and wake, a homeostatic component affects sleep propensity. Whereas the circadian process is relatively independent of prior sleep or wake, the homeostatic process on the other hand is sensitive to the sleep-wake history [14]. The sleep homeostatic process increases sleep propensity during wakefulness until sleep is initiated. Sleep propensity declines during sleep. The biological mechanisms underlying sleep homeostasis are less well characterized than the mechanisms underlying circadian regulation, but likely involve astrocyte-dependent adenosine accumulation [15, 16].

Some types of insomnia may involve altered homeostatic regulation of sleep propensity [17].

Sleep-permissive and wake-promoting conditions

Although the regulation of sleep and wakefulness has been well modeled by interacting circadian and homeostatic processes in lab studies, additional sleep-permissive and wake-promoting conditions have been largely overlooked [18]. Most people sleep best lying in bed, with the lights off and the curtains drawn, in a thermally agreeable, safe and relatively quiet environment. These parameters (posture, temperature, environmental light and sound, fear) moderate the expression of sleep and wake. Whether altered sensitivity to sleep-permissive and wake-promoting conditions might be involved in the sleep complaints of some types of insomnia remains virtually unexplored [1921].

Treatment of insomnia

Even though the underlying causes of insomnia are at present insufficiently understood, one of the most common treatments provided by primary caregivers (mainly general practitioners) is sedative or hypnotic drugs mostly benzodiazepines or benzodiazepine receptor antagonists [1]. Unfortunately, pharmacological treatments have a high prevalence of adverse effects. Daytime drowsiness, risk of abuse and addiction, and rebound insomnia on withdrawal are a few examples of these side effects [22]. Pharmacological treatment is therefore not recommended [23].

In the past decades, two other interventions have been developed. The first intervention is cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia (CBT-I). It has similar or even better and more prolonged outcomes than pharmacological treatment, and lacks the negative side effects [24]. A second, less evolved, but promising type of intervention is chronobiological treatment (CT). Chronobiological treatments aim to enhance regular and appropriately timed input to the circadian timing system to support its role in sleep regulation, by use of bright light, body warming or physical activity (for example, [21, 25, 26], for a review see [27]). These manipulations may become more important with increasing age, to counteract age-related changes in the functional neuroanatomy of the biological clock of the brain [28].

Cognitive behavioral therapy

Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia (CBT-I) is a combination of cognitive and behavioral treatments. CBT-I usually includes stimulus control (that is, association of bed with sleeping), sleep restriction (that is, restrict an individual’s time in bed to average sleep time), relaxation training, cognitive therapy (that is, diminish misconceptions about sleep) and sleep hygiene (for example, general guidelines about behavioral and environmental factors that influence sleep) [29]. CBT-I has been shown to be effective in numerous randomized clinical trials [24, 2934]. A problem facing implementation on a large scale is the lack of skilled therapists.

Chronobiological treatment

Three chronobiological manipulations have been applied successfully and will be discussed below.

Bright Light (BL)

Bright light, including natural daylight, entrains the circadian rhythm [35]. Retinal ganglion cells projecting to the SCN through the retinohypothalamic tract (RHT) inform the circadian timing system about the intensity of light exposure. The SCN uses this information to entrain its intrinsic rhythmic activity to the 24-h light–dark cycle [10]. Studies in humans show that phase shifts can be imposed by manipulation of the light–dark cycle [35, 36]. A phase advance can be induced by means of bright light given in the morning, especially closely after the body core temperature minimum; a phase delay can be induced with light applied in the interval from late afternoon to just prior to the body core temperature maximum [35, 37, 38]. The effect of light on sleep may not be limited to changing the sleep phase, as suggested by studies that found enhanced sleep quality in the absence of clear rhythm shifts [25, 39].

Body and skin warming (BW)

Core body temperature shows a clear 24-h rhythm. In normal sleepers with a sleep period between 23:00 and 7:00, the temperature peaks between 18:00 and 20:00 h and has its minimum between 4:00 and 5:00 h [11]. Apparently, the connection between sleep or vigilance and core body temperature is strong. We sleep when core body temperature is low and are awake when it is high [18]. In contrast, skin temperature is increased during the sleep period because of increased skin blood flow in response to a supine posture [40]. Increased skin blood flow promotes heat loss, resulting in a lower core body temperature. Romeijn et al. [18] reviewed the effect of manipulation of skin temperature on vigilance, and they explain in more detail the underlying processes. Several studies that applied water-perfused thermo suits to increase skin temperature slightly within the thermoneutral zone showed that this manipulation enhances sleep propensity [18]. This effect has not only been demonstrated in healthy adults, but also in people with insomnia [20, 21]. A more practical and home-applicable version of this temperature manipulation is to make use of the body’s thermoregulatory mechanisms and warm the body prior to sleep by mild physical activity or taking a hot bath [41]. About two hours later, core body temperature has returned to baseline, while skin temperature is still somewhat elevated. This pre-sleep body warming approach has been used with success in insomnia [42].

Physical Activity (PA)

As diurnal mammals, humans are active during the day and sleep at night. The rest-activity profile of people with a very inactive lifestyle, for example sedentary elderly, shows a small 24-h amplitude. Several studies evaluated the effect of enhancing physical activity or exercising on this amplitude and on sleep [26, 4351]. Two possible mechanisms have been proposed to be involved in the effect of enhanced physical activity on sleep. The thermoregulatory hypothesis proposes that exercise exerts an effect on sleep through initially increasing core body temperature. This subsequently triggers heat loss to decrease the temperature, which is conducive to sleep. The body restoration hypothesis proposes that exercise depletes energy levels and promotes sleep because it increases the need to activate restorative mechanisms. In 2000, Driver & Taylor reviewed the studies done up until that year [46]. Unfortunately, most studies use good sleepers. Their conclusion was that exercise of moderate intensity and focusing on endurance rather than peak intensity, was most beneficial for perceived sleep quality. Tanaka et al. [51] showed that exercise is most effective around 17:00 h, supporting the thermoregulatory hypothesis. Benloucif et al. [43] however found equal effects of morning and evening exercise on subjective sleep quality. More recently, Passos et al. [50] summarized studies using exercise in primary insomnia. This review demonstrated improvement in sleep quality. Additionally, they discuss possible explanations for the acute and chronic effects of physical activity on primary insomnia [50].

Optimizing treatment and accessibility

Whereas the combination of CT and CBT-I may be more effective than their use in isolation, and has successfully been applied in only one study [30, 52], no prior study addressed the relative contribution of the different interventions. More research is therefore needed to compare effectiveness of different combinations of therapies; one of the aims of the present study.

A problem facing implementation of CBT-I is the shortage of skilled therapists. Internet-based CBT-I may alleviate this problem [53]. Internet-based therapy is highly self-guided, structured and personalized. Knowledge-based technology renders a personalized concept consult, which can be adjusted by therapist for situations that cannot be pre-programmed. This saves a lot of time for a therapist and subsequently may help to solve the discrepancy between care needed and care available [33, 34], thus allowing for large-scale implementation.

Another opportunity for the optimization of treatment is based on the idea that the people suffering from insomnia most likely represent a heterogeneous mix of subtypes, with different underlying causes and expected treatment response. These subtypes may not only be distinguished by type of sleep problems. Other factors such as personality traits, medical complaints and medical history, the ability to perceive comfort [21, 5456] may represent variables that differ between the proposed subtypes as well. Our research group commenced a large-scale study using web-based assessment of questionnaires to collect data on these variables (the Netherlands Sleep Registry, NSR, www.sleepregistry.org). The use of latent class analysis on these data will allow for the data-driven detection of multivariate subtypes, calculation of subtype probability for participants according to this data-driven subtype nosology, and evaluation of differential treatment responses, depending on the subtype probability.

The present study aims to evaluate the possibility to optimize insomnia treatment along the principles of personalized and stratified medicine, by making the first step towards the development of a protocol for individualized optimal treatment for specific subtypes of insomnia, which each may show different responsiveness to CBT-I and the different CT manipulations.

Objectives

The objectives of this study are as follows:

1. To evaluate the relative effectiveness of internet-based CBT-I, three different types of CT (light, temperature and physical activity), and the combined application of CBT-I and each of the CT’s, as compared to placebo treatment, on [1] subjective sleep efficiency (primary outcome), [2] subjective daytime functioning, [3] actigraphic sleep estimates, and [4] cost effectiveness.

2. To evaluate whether insomnia subtype moderates the effectiveness of the individual and the combined treatments.

3. To evaluate whether the effect of CBT-I is enhanced by prior CT.

Methods/Design

Trial design

The study is a double blind, randomized, placebo controlled trial. Aiming at 160 participants who complete the trial, with a dropout rate of 10 %, 180 participants will be recruited, who will be randomly assigned to one of eight limbs in a repeated measures design with two factors (CBT-I and CT), of respectively 2 levels (active CBT-I treatment versus waitlist monitoring) and 4 levels (light, physical activity, body warming and placebo) (see Fig. 1). Covariate-adaptive randomization aims at an equal representation of insomnia subtypes within each treatment limb. Assessment will be performed at baseline (week 0), after a 4-week treatment period (week 5), and after a second 4-week treatment/monitoring period (week 10).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart of the trial design and outcome assessments. In the recruitment phase, participants fill out a questionnaire to assess their eligibility to participate. This questionnaire includes the Duke Structured Interview for Sleep Disorder Diagnosis (30 min). During weeks 0, 5 and 10, participants fill out questionnaires and sleep diaries and will wear an actigraph for subjective and objective sleep estimates, respectively. The diary is continued in all other weeks to support cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT). During weeks 1, 4, 6 and 9, participants will wear actigraphs for objective measures of compliance to chronobiological treatment

Participants

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Participants will be included that meet the criteria of an insomnia disorder as defined in the DSM-V. Furthermore, participants have to be between 18 and 70 years old. Because a meta-analysis [57] indicated that insomnia is more prevalent in females (odds ratio (OR) = 1.41), we aim for inclusion of 60 % females and 40 % males.

This study follows the Helsinki Declaration’s principles, meaning that all patients sign a written informed consent stating that participation is voluntary and that participation can be withdrawn at any time, without any negative consequences concerning their current or future treatment. Approval was obtained on 21 May 2013 from the medical ethical committee of the VU University medical center in The Netherlands (Protocol 12/472). The trial is registered at the Netherlands National Trial Register (NTR4010) [58].

Participants are recruited through the Netherlands Sleep Registry and (social) media. Interested candidates fill out a screening questionnaire regarding health status and insomnia diagnostic criteria (relevant items of Duke Structured Interview for Sleep Disorders, DSISD) [59], level of physical activity [60], light exposure (by asking about habitual hours spent outside during daylight hours) and bathing habits (frequency and duration of bathing/showering). Participants will be excluded for the following reasons:

  1. 1.

    their estimated baseline level and timing of activity, bright light or body warming are already similar to the CT interventions planned;

  2. 2.

    they report an eye disease incompatible with light treatment (aphakia or retinopathy), or cardiovascular or movement disorder incompatible with the exercise treatment;

  3. 3.

    they report that they are currently diagnosed with a psychiatric or neurological disorder;

  4. 4.

    they are shift workers, since the treatment protocol does not allow for alternative schedules, and their sleeping problems may not be due solely to insomnia; or

  5. 5.

    they use sleep medication regularly, unless they are willing and able to restrict their usage to a maximum use of twice a week, at least 1 month prior to enrollment.

Randomization

Participants who meet the criteria and sign informed consent will be randomly assigned to CBT-I or to the waitlist and to one of the four CT conditions (including one placebo). Simple randomization will be applied to the first batch of 48 participants. During the staggered entry of subsequent participants, covariate-adaptive randomization [61] will be applied in order to maintain balanced groups throughout the study. The covariates are age, sex, use of non-sleep related medication and time of year. Since some covariates are continuous rather than categorical, randomization will be done following the method proposed by Frane [62]. This method temporarily assigns each new participant one by one to each treatment group and obtains a P value of the test for between-group differences for each of the covariates for that assignment, using analysis of variance (ANOVA) for continuous variables and a X2 test for categorical variables. This renders a P value for each of the four covariates for each of the eight possible assignments. The lowest of the four P values thus represents the least balanced covariate in that assignment situation. These lowest, or minimum, P values per assignment will then be used to make the actual assignment to one of the treatment conditions, where a participant will be assigned to the group for which this minimum P value is highest, thus resulting in a group assignment that keeps imbalance over groups as small as possible. If group sizes become unbalanced (that is, differing by more than five participants), the Frane method will be applied only to the six smallest groups instead of all eight. All randomization will be done using R [63]. Randomization will be scripted, so actual group assignment is automated.

Blinding and expectation assessment

A patient information letter explains the four CT manipulations (including placebo CT) and CBT-I). All treatments in the study are presented to the participants as possibly effective. It is not possible to blind participants to the different treatment conditions. Since all information is given via email or through the postal service, there will not be a blinding of instructors. However, CBT-I counselors are instructed not to correspond about the expected outcome of CT treatments, and participants will be asked not to mention their CT condition in possible correspondence with the CBT-I counselor. When participants, however, do reveal their condition, the CBT-I counselor will be replaced. In order to secure blinded data analysis, information regarding treatment conditions will be coded. Only once data processing is finished and the dataset is finalized, will the code will be broken. The analyst (KD) will not have access to the key document. Because all outcome assessments are either self-reported through the internet or obtained from recording devices, blinding of the assessors in not applicable. Judgments of the participant on expected effectiveness will be assessed at T0, T1 and T2 using a 7-point Likert-scale for each of the treatments. After randomization, at T0, participants will be asked about the expectation regarding their assigned CT and CBT-I. After treatment, at T1 and T2, participants will be asked to what extent their sleep problems have changed compared to baseline and to what extent they attribute this change to the treatment.

Study settings

All measurements and therapies are conducted at the participants’ homes, using the Internet [64, 65]. Treatment devices are sent to their homes. Participants are informed on the types of treatment used in the study and the objective of comparing them.

Interventions

Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia

All participants will receive internet-based, personalized cognitive behavioral treatment for insomnia provided through the Somnio website for 4 weeks. The Somnio Internet therapy uses knowledge-based technology to prepare a consult and support the therapist to apply the protocol in a consistent and comprehensive way [66]. The personally assigned CBT-I therapist can adjust each consult if needed. CBT-I will consist of four consults, one every Monday morning. Every morning and evening, participants fill out the Dutch online version of the Consensus Sleep Diary [67]. The first consult will be based on the sleep diary data from the previous week, in combination with the person’s beliefs and attitude towards sleep, as assessed during week 0 by the Dysfunctional Beliefs and Attitudes towards Sleep (DBAS) questionnaire [68]. Sleep diary data of the consecutive weeks will be used to determine which cognitive and behavioral components are emphasized during the treatment [66]. A more detailed description of CBT-I has been provided earlier in this article.

Chronobiological treatment

Physical Activity

At enrolment, participants are asked to fill out questionnaires to assess their health status as well as the habitual level and timing of activity [60]. The answers on an extended Baecke questionnaire will provide the necessary information to determine the specific personalized implementation if they are randomized to the physical activity condition. More specifically, in the active treatment limb, the most intense physical activity (PA) that participants report to habitually maintain for at least half an hour (for example, (walking, running, cycling) will be (re)scheduled to be performed daily for half an hour preferably starting three hours before ideal bedtime, and never ending closer than two hours prior to ideal bedtime. The physical activity will thus at no point exceed the participants usual duration and intensity, but will be daily and set to a specific time of the day.

Body Warming

For body warming (BW), participants randomized to the temperature condition are instructed to take a warm bath daily for half an hour, starting 3 hours before the ideal bedtime and never ending closer than 2 hours prior to ideal bedtime. The physical activity and temperature manipulation procedures will result in elevated skin temperature at bedtime, which can enhance sleep onset [18]. If the manipulations would be done closer to bedtime, core body temperature would not have returned to baseline at bedtime and possibly interfere with sleep [18, 19].

Bright Light

Participants randomized to the bright light (BL) condition will receive a Philips goLITE BLU light device (HF3220/01, Philips Consumer Lifestyle, Drachten, The Netherlands). They will be instructed to install the light on a table facing a window to minimize glare by reducing contrast between relatively small bright light source and the background. The light will be set on the side, within the range of vision, but not straight across the participant. This will reduce strain on the eyes. They will be asked to sit facing the light in close proximity for half an hour at a fixed time each morning within an hour after habitual wake-up time, for example during breakfast.

Deactivated Ionizer

Many randomized controlled trials testing the effect of morning bright light on seasonal affective disorders (SAD), compare bright light with negative air ionization [6972]. These studies show that morning High Density Negative Ionization (HDNI) is as effective as morning bright light treatment. Negative air ionization has been shown to positively affect cognitive performance and depression [73]. The application has not been evaluated for its possible effect on insomnia. In one study on SAD, a deactivated ionizer (DI) was used as placebo condition [74]. The ionizer was modified to suggest normal functioning, as indicated by airflow, while negative ion production had been deactivated. Treatment outcome expectancy for the (deactivated) negative ionizer was equal to that of BL. In the present study, participants randomized to the placebo treatment will therefore receive a likewise deactivated ionizer (DI) device (Ionic Air Purifier, XJ-2100, Shanghai Neo.Tec Electron Co., Ltd, Shanghai, China). Participants will be instructed to install the device on a table where they can sit in close proximity to it for half an hour each morning at their earliest convenience after ideal wake-up time, for example during breakfast. This placebo has been applied successfully in several studies in the USA [69, 74], but not yet in the Netherlands, making it unlikely to be recognized as a placebo.

Assessments and outcomes

To assess subjective sleep parameters as well as daytime complaints, participants will be keeping a diary in the morning and evening for the entire 11-week protocol. Sleep is assessed in the morning using the Dutch version of the Consensus Sleep Diary [67]. Primary outcome, sleep efficiency (the percentage of time slept during the time in bed for sleep), is calculated from the sleep diary variables as follows:

$$ SE\kern0.5em =\kern0.5em \frac{\left( LightOn\kern0.5em -\kern0.5em LightOff\right)\kern0.5em -\kern0.5em SOL\kern0.5em -\kern0.5em WASO\kern0.5em -\kern0.5em EMA}{LightOn-\kern0.5em LightOff} $$

where SE is sleep efficiency; LightsOn is the moment people indicated they stop trying to sleep, calculated from their final wake up time and time they stayed in bed to try to fall asleep again; LightsOff is the time people turn off the lights and/or disengage from activities to go to sleep; SOL is sleep onset latency; WASO is the time awake after sleep onset; and EMA is early morning awakening, or the time people spend trying to fall back asleep after final awakening.

Daytime functioning is assessed in the evening using a dedicated questionnaire on all complaints listed in the DSM-V and the International Classification of Sleep Disorders, 2nd edition (ICSD-2). During week 0, participants will fill out the complete Duke Structured Interview for Sleep Disorders in order to have a complete overview of sleep complaints and other medical and psychological complaints they currently have or have had in the past.

During weeks 0, 5 and 10, participants will fill out a comprehensive set of questionnaires:

  1. 1.

    Expectations or subjective outcome of the selected treatments;

  2. 2.

    Health-related costs during the last 4 weeks (Trimbos/iMTA questionnaire for Costs associated with Psychiatric Illness: TiC-P) [75];

  3. 3.

    Functional health (Quality of Life Questionnaire, Short Form 36, SF-36) [76];

  4. 4.

    Depression and anxiety (Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, HADS) [77];

  5. 5.

    Mood (Positive Affect, Negative Affect Scale, PANAS) [78];

  6. 6.

    Experience of pleasure and comfort (Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale, TEPS) [79];

  7. 7.

    Thoughts during resting state (Amsterdam Resting State Questionnaire, ARSQ) [55]

  8. 8.

    Hyperarousal. Because this is considered a key feature of insomnia, it is queried using four different scales; the Arousal Predisposition Scale (APS) [80]; the Hyper Arousal Scale (HAS) [81]; the adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS) [82]; and the Pre-Sleep Arousal Scale (PSAS) [83];

  9. 9.

    Insomnia severity (Insomnia Severity Index, ISI) [84];

  10. 10.

    Beliefs and attitudes regarding sleep (DBAS) [68];

  11. 11.

    Sleep effort (Glasgow Sleep Effort Scale, GSES) [85];

  12. 12.

    Sleep self-efficacy (Sleep Self-Efficacy Scale, SSES) [86];

  13. 13.

    Sleep locus of control (SLOC) [87].

As shown in Fig. 1, ambulatory recordings will be performed during the baseline week and the first week of treatment (weeks 0 and 1), the last week of the CT treatment period and the first week following completion (weeks 4 and 5), and during weeks 9 and 10. During these weeks, participants will continuously wear a wrist actigraph to estimate sleep and light exposure (Philips Actiwatch Spectrum, Philips Respironics, Murrysville, PA, USA) and an accelerometer worn on the trunk to estimate energy expenditure (Philips DirectLife, DL8700/01, Philips, Eindhoven, The Netherlands).

Compliance

CBT-I

Compliance with some aspects of CBT-I, for example sleep restriction and stimulus control, can be monitored using the sleep diary combined with the actigraphic sleep estimates.

CT

Compliance with the chronobiological treatments will be monitored by means of questions added to the sleep diaries. Moreover, objective measures of compliance for BL and PA can be derived from ambulatory monitoring of physical activity and light exposure by the Actiwatch Spectrum and DirectLife devices. Participants randomized to BL are instructed to expose the Actiwatch Spectrum to the light when sitting in front of the goLite BLU. Participants assigned to BW receive a digital bath thermometer (type: Lotus, Béaba, Oyonnax Cedex, France) and are asked to register temperature of the bathing water to monitor compliance with the body warming treatment.

Outcome measures

The primary outcome measure is the change from week 0 to week 5 in subjective sleep efficiency derived from the sleep diary. Secondary subjective sleep estimates include difficulties falling asleep, difficulties maintaining sleep, early morning awakening and non-restorative sleep. A secondary outcome regarding daytime consequences is the average rating of evening diary ratings on eighteen major daytime functioning complaints. Ancillary analyses on persistence of treatment use the same variables obtained in week 10.

Actigraphically estimated objective sleep parameters (sleep efficiency, sleep onset latency, sleep duration, wake after sleep onset, and the average durations of uninterrupted periods of sleep and wakefulness) are secondary outcome measures meant to complement the subjective evaluations. These are calculated from the ActiWatch Spectrum recordings using the Respironics Actiware software (version 5.71.0, Philips Respironics, Murrysville, PA, USA). Given the variability in subjective [88] and objective [89] sleep estimates, they will be assessed for 7 subsequent days at each assessment. Additional questionnaires, assessed in weeks 0, 5 and 10, will provide secondary outcome measures (see the list above). Specifically, the direct and indirect medical costs (TiC-P) will be included.

Effect moderation by subtype score

Preliminary results of the Latent Class Analysis on comprehensive psychometric data of a large sample of participants of the Netherlands Sleep Registry suggests the existence of four different subtypes of insomnia, each with about equal prevalence. From the extensive number of questionnaires on traits these volunteers filled out, we are presently selecting the minimal subset of questions required to obtain good estimates of the a posteriori probabilities of a participant to belong to each one of the four classes. These items will be included in a Sleep Subtype Survey that will be assessed in all participants of the present RCT. The resulting four Insomnia Subtype Probabilities (ISP) will be included in analyses aimed at evaluating treatment effect moderation by each of the four insomnia subtype probabilities.

Statistical analysis

Data reduction and reporting

The data of qualitative variables (for example, gender, type of treatment, and insomnia subtype) will be presented as incidence rates (number and percent). The data of continuous variables (for example, age, measures derived from sleep diaries, questionnaires and actigraphy) will be presented by measures of central tendency (that is, mean and median) and dispersion (that is, standard deviation and range).

Treatment effects and moderation estimates

Mixed effect models will be applied to estimate all time by treatment effects, time by treatment interaction effects and effect moderation by the individual’s insomnia subtype probabilities. For each outcome measure, all effects will be estimated simultaneously in one single linear regression equation. The primary outcome measure of sleep efficiency will be illustrated below. The diary and actigraphy based outcome measures have a two-level hierarchical data-structure: repeated measures of 14 diary inputs i across T0 and T1 are nested within subjects j. The 14*160 = 2,240 data points can be used to estimate simultaneously all effects ß using the following mixed effect model:

$$ \begin{array}{l}\mathrm{S}{\mathrm{E}}_{\mathrm{ij}}\kern0.5em =\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_{0\mathrm{i}\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_1*{\mathrm{Time}}_{\mathrm{ij}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_2*\mathrm{C}\mathrm{B}\mathrm{T}\hbox{-} {\mathrm{I}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_3*\mathrm{B}{\mathrm{L}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_4*\mathrm{P}{\mathrm{A}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_5*\mathrm{B}{\mathrm{W}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_6*\mathrm{IS}{\mathrm{P}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_7*\mathrm{T}\mathrm{i}\mathrm{m}{\mathrm{e}}_{\mathrm{ij}}*\mathrm{IS}{\mathrm{P}}_{\mathrm{j}}+\\ {}\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{8}}*\mathbf{C}\mathbf{B}\mathbf{T}\hbox{-} {\mathbf{I}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_9*\mathrm{C}\mathrm{B}\mathrm{T}\hbox{-} \mathrm{I}*\mathrm{IS}{\mathrm{P}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{10}}*\mathbf{C}\mathbf{B}\mathbf{T}\mathbf{\hbox{-} }{\mathbf{I}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}*\mathbf{I}\mathbf{S}\mathbf{P}+\\ {}\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{11}}*\mathbf{B}{\mathbf{L}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_{12}*\mathrm{B}\mathrm{L}*\mathrm{IS}{\mathrm{P}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{13}}*\mathbf{B}{\mathbf{L}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}*\mathbf{I}\mathbf{S}{\mathbf{P}}_{\mathbf{j}}+\\ {}\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{14}}*\mathbf{P}{\mathbf{A}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_{15}*\mathrm{P}\mathrm{A}*\mathrm{IS}{\mathrm{P}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{16}}*\mathbf{P}{\mathbf{A}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}*\mathbf{I}\mathbf{S}{\mathbf{P}}_{\mathbf{j}}+\\ {}\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{17}}*\mathbf{B}{\mathbf{W}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_{18}*\mathrm{B}\mathrm{W}*\mathrm{IS}{\mathrm{P}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{19}}*\mathbf{B}{\mathbf{W}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}*\mathbf{I}\mathbf{S}{\mathbf{P}}_{\mathbf{j}}+\\ {}\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_{20}*\mathrm{C}\mathrm{B}\mathrm{T}\hbox{-} {\mathrm{I}}_{\mathrm{j}}*\mathrm{B}{\mathrm{L}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{21}}*\mathbf{C}\mathbf{B}\mathbf{T}\mathbf{\hbox{-} }{\mathbf{I}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{B}{\mathbf{L}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_{22}*\mathrm{C}\mathrm{B}\mathrm{T}\hbox{-} {\mathrm{I}}_{\mathrm{j}}*\mathrm{B}{\mathrm{L}}_{\mathrm{j}}*\mathrm{IS}{\mathrm{P}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{23}}*\mathbf{C}\mathbf{B}\mathbf{T}\hbox{-} \\ {}\kern0.5em {\mathbf{I}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{B}{\mathbf{L}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}*\mathbf{I}\mathbf{S}\mathbf{P}+\\ {}\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_{24}*\mathrm{C}\mathrm{B}\mathrm{T}\hbox{-} {\mathrm{I}}_{\mathrm{j}}*\mathrm{P}{\mathrm{A}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{25}}*\mathbf{C}\mathbf{B}\mathbf{T}\mathbf{\hbox{-} }{\mathbf{I}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{P}{\mathbf{A}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_{26}*\mathrm{C}\mathrm{B}\mathrm{T}\hbox{-} {\mathrm{I}}_{\mathrm{j}}*\mathrm{P}{\mathrm{A}}_{\mathrm{j}}*\mathrm{IS}{\mathrm{P}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{27}}*\mathbf{C}\mathbf{B}\mathbf{T}\mathbf{\hbox{-}}\\ {}\kern0.5em {\mathbf{I}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{P}{\mathbf{A}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}*\mathbf{I}\mathbf{S}\mathbf{P}\mathbf{j}+\\ {}\kern0.5em {\mathrm{\ss}}_{28}*\mathrm{C}\mathrm{B}\mathrm{T}\hbox{-} {\mathrm{I}}_{\mathrm{j}}*\mathrm{B}{\mathrm{W}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{29}}*\mathbf{C}\mathbf{B}\mathbf{T}\mathbf{\hbox{-} }{\mathbf{I}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{B}{\mathbf{W}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}\kern0.5em +{\mathrm{\ss}}_{30}*\mathrm{C}\mathrm{B}\mathrm{T}\hbox{-} {\mathrm{I}}_{\mathrm{j}}*\mathrm{B}{\mathrm{W}}_{\mathrm{j}}*\mathrm{IS}{\mathrm{P}}_{\mathrm{j}}\kern0.5em +\kern0.5em {\mathbf{\ss}}_{\mathbf{31}}*\mathbf{C}\mathbf{B}\mathbf{T}\mathbf{\hbox{-}}\\ {}\kern0.5em {\mathbf{I}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{B}{\mathbf{W}}_{\mathbf{j}}*\mathbf{T}\mathbf{i}\mathbf{m}{\mathbf{e}}_{\mathbf{ij}}*\mathbf{I}\mathbf{S}{\mathbf{P}}_{\mathbf{j}}\end{array} $$

In this regression equation, SE = sleep efficiency; ß0ij = intercept; time denotes the post intervention (T1) versus baseline (T0) assessment week; CBT = cognitive behavior therapy assignment; BL = bright light assignment; PA = physical activity assignment; BW = body warming assignment; ISP = insomnia subtype probability; and ß1 to ß31 denote effect estimates. The effects and terms of interest are shown in bold font. For example, line 2 shows in bold font the overall treatment by time interaction effect ß 8 of CBT-I versus waitlist and the moderation of this effect ß 10 by one’s insomnia subtype probability. Lines 3 to 5 show the corresponding effect estimates for BL, PA and BW interventions, and lines 6 to 8 the for their interaction with CBT-I. For clarity, the regression equation shows only one of the four expected ISPs. Wald tests will be used to evaluate the significance of the effect estimates. Statistical significance is thresholded at p = 0.05. The simultaneous estimation of all effects of interest in a single regression model compensates for multiple comparisons. Mixed effect model analyses will be implemented in MLwiN (version 2.02, Centre for Multilevel Modelling, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK.) and (R: A language and evironment for statistical computing, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.).

Power and sample size

Statistical power estimates

The statistical power of mixed effect regression models with seven repeated measures (diaries) both before and after intervention have been calculated according to the equations given by Twisk, page 280 [90], similar to those calculations made in a previous repeated measures RCT that we published [91]. Statistical power calculations on repeated measures require an estimate of the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC, that is, within-subject). We estimated the ICC for the primary outcome measure of subjective sleep efficiency from the 9-day sleep diaries [67] that we recently assessed through the internet for 27 people suffering from insomnia according to an Insomnia Severity Index ≥10 [92]. In agreement with the robust observation that night-by-night sleep variability is an intrinsic part of the insomnia symptomatology (reviewed in [88]), we found only a moderate ICC of 0.27, supporting the use of repeated assessment of the primary outcome measure to increase statistical power of the design.

Given this moderate ICC of 0.27, seven pre-assessments at T0, and seven post-assessments at T1, the 160 completing participants provide a power 1-beta of 0.80 to allow for the detection, at a significance of alpha = 0.05, of even small to medium main effects of CBT-I (minimal detectable difference d = 0.27 standard deviations), of each of the three CTs (d = 0.31) and of their interaction with CBT-I (d = 0.41). The minimally detectable difference in the time by treatment interaction between any two types of CT is small to medium as well (d = 0.38).

There is unfortunately no readily available methodology to estimate the minimally detectable moderation effects of continuous variables (insomnia subtype probabilities) on a time by treatment interaction effect on a continuous outcome variable (sleep efficiency). In order to obtain a conservative estimate of minimally detectable moderation effects, we can dichotomize the continuous insomnia subtype probability variable by means of a median split, effectively generating two equal groups. The design then becomes one of three factors, each with two levels: time (T1 versus T0), treatment group (Treated versus Untreated on the specific intervention) and insomnia subtype (high versus low probability) and provides the ratio of available data in the subgroup or interest relative to the other available data, to be used once more in the equations given by Twisk, page 280 [90]. According to this conservative approach of mapping the continuous insomnia subtype probabilities onto a dichotomous variable, the 160 completing participants provide a power 1-beta of 0.80 to allow for the detection, at a significance of alpha = 0.05: of a small to medium moderation effects of insomnia subtype (1) on the time by CBT-I treatment interaction (d = 0.31); (2) on the time by each CT treatment interaction (d = 0.41); and (3) on the difference in the time by treatment interaction between any two types of CT (d = 0.44). The smallest detectable moderation effect of insomnia subtype on the time by combined CBT-I and CT interaction is medium sized (d = 0.55). We follow the convention on effect size given by Cohen [93], who suggested consideration of an effect size d of about 0.2 as “small” and an effect size of about 0.5 as “medium.” With an estimated dropout of 10 %, we will recruit 180 volunteers. The expected 160 completers thus allow for the detection of small to medium effect sizes.

Discussion

The ultimate goal of this study is to develop a personalized treatment protocol for insomnia disorder. Primary caregivers could use this protocol to decide on the primary choice treatment for each individual patient. Assessment of insomnia subtype probabilities using the Sleep Subtype Survey results in four ISPs, which can be used to determine the expected treatment outcome of each of four treatments, their combinations and their simultaneous versus sequential application for the individual patient.

Since all treatments used in this study are noninvasive, easily applicable at home and require only the use of a computer and Internet (CBT-I) and/or a readily available therapy light (BL), the implementation of treatments is straightforward, low-cost and scalable.

The current study addresses multiple opportunities to optimize and personalize treatment of insomnia disorder. First, no prior study has evaluated the relative effectiveness of CBT-I, the three different forms of CT, and their simultaneous or sequential combinations. Second, the moderation of outcome by insomnia subtype probability follows the principles of personalized and stratified medicine. Treatment effects with subtype-specificity may come to light that would have remained hidden in the unrecognized heterogeneity of the samples.

Trial status

Recruitment started in October 2013 and is ongoing. In January 2014, the first participants enrolled. Currently, 60 participants have completed the protocol.

Abbreviations

APS:

arousal predisposition scale

ARSQ:

Amsterdam resting state questionnaire

ASRS:

ADHD self-report scale

BL:

bright light treatment

BW:

body warming treatment

CBT:

cognitive behavioral therapy

CBT-I:

cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia

CSD:

consensus sleep diary

CT:

chronobiological treatment

DI:

deactivated ionizer

DBAS:

dysfunctional beliefs and attitudes towards sleep

DSISD:

Duke’s Structured Interview for Sleep Disorders

DSM-V:

diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th edition

EMA:

early morning awakening

GSES:

Glasgow sleep efficiency scale

HADS:

hospital anxiety and depression scale

HAS:

hyperarousal scale

HDNI:

high density negative ionization

ICC:

intraclass correlation coefficient

ICSD-2:

international classification of sleep disorders, 2nd edition

ID:

insomnia disorder

ISI:

insomnia severity index

ISP:

insomnia subtype probability

OR:

odds ratio

PA:

physical activity treatment

PANAS:

positive affect, negative affect scale

PSAS:

pre-sleep arousal scale

SCN:

suprachiasmatic nucleus

SE:

sleep efficiency

SF-36:

short form 36

SLOC:

sleep locus of control

SOL:

sleep onset latency

SSES:

sleep self-efficacy scale

TEPS:

temporal experience of pleasure scale

TEPS:

temporal experience of pleasure scale

TiC-P:

Trimbos/iMTA questionnaire for costs associated with psychiatric illness

WASO:

wake after sleep onset

References

  1. Morin CM, Benca RM. Chronic insomnia. Lancet. 2012;379:1129–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Morin CM, LeBlanc M, Daley M, Gregoire JP, Mérette C. Epidemiology of insomnia: prevalence, self-help treatments, consultations, and determinants of help-seeking behaviors. Sleep Med. 2006;7:123–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Ohayon MM. Epidemiology of insomnia: what we know and what we still need to learn. Sleep Med Rev. 2002;6:97–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. American Psychiatric Association DSM, Force T. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  5. Fortier-Brochu É, Beaulieu-Bonneau S, Ivers H, Morin CM. Insomnia and daytime cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2012;16:83–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Buysse DJ, Thompsom W, Scott J, Franzen PL, Germain A, Hall M, et al. Daytime symptoms in primary insomnia: a prospective analysis using ecological momentary assessment. Sleep Med. 2006;8:198–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Baglioni C, Battagliese G, Feige B, Spiegelhalder K, Nissen C, Voderholzer U, et al. Insomnia as a predictor of depression: A meta-analytic evaluation of longitudinal epidemiological studies. J Affect Disord. 2011;135:10–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Schwartz S, Anderson WM, Cole SR, Cornoni-Huntley J, Hays JC, Blazer D. Insomnia and heart disease: A review of epidemiologic studies. J Psychosom Res. 1999;47:313–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Daley M, Morin CM, LeBlanc M, Gregoire JP, Savard J, Baillargeon L. Insomnia and its relationship to health-care utilization, work absenteeism, prductivity and accidents. Sleep Med. 2009;10:427–38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Dibner C, Schibler U, Albrecht U. The Mammalian Circadian Timing System: Organization and Coordination of Central and Peripheral Clocks. Annu Rev Physiol. 2010;72:517–49.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Lack LC, Wright HR. Treating chronobiological components of chronic insomnia. Sleep Med. 2007;8:637–44. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2006.10.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Lack LC, Gradisar M, Someren EJWV, Wright HR, Lushington K. The relationship between insomnia and body temperatures. Sleep Med Rev. 2008;12:307–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Van Veen MM, Kooij JJS, Boonstra AM, Gordijn MCM, Van Someren EJW. Delayed circadian rhythm in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and chronic sleep-onset insomnia. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:1091–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Daan S, Beersma DGM, Borbély AA. Timing of human sleep: recovery process gated by a circadian pacemaker. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 1984;15:161–78.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Halassa MM, Florian C, Fellin T, Munoz JR, Lee SY, Abel T, et al. Astrocytic modulation of sleep homeostasis and cognitive consequences of sleep loss. Neuron. 2009;61:213–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.024.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Porkka-Heiskanen T, Strecker RE, Thakkar M, Bjorkum AA, Greene RW, McCarley RW. Adenosine: a mediator of the sleep-inducing effects of prolonged wakefulness. Science. 1997;276:1265–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Pigeon WR, Perlis ML. Sleep homeostasis in primary insomnia. Sleep Med Rev. 2006;10:247–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Romeijn N, Raymann RJEM, Most E, Te Lindert B, Van Der Meijden WP, Fronzek R, et al. Sleep, vigilance and thermosensitivity. Eur J Physiol. 2012;463:169–76.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Raymann RJEM, Swaab DF, Someren EJWV. Skin temperature and sleep-onset latency: Changes with age and insomnia. Physiol Behav. 2007;90:257–66.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Raymann RJEM, Swaab DF, Van Someren EJW. Skin deep: enhanced sleep depth by cutaneous temperature manipulation. Brain. 2008;131:500–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Raymann RJEM, Van Someren EJW. Diminished capability to recognize the optimal temperature for sleep initiation may contribute to poor sleep in elderly people. Sleep. 2008;31:1301–9.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Benca RM. Diagnosis and Treatment of Chronic Insomnia: A Review. Psychiatr Serv. 2005;56:332–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. National Institutes of Health. National Institutes of Health State of the Science Conference Statement on manifestations and management of chronic insomnia in adults June 13–15, 2005. Sleep. 2005;28:1049–57.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Morin CM, Vallières A, Guay B, Ivers H, Savard J, Mérette C, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy, singly and combined with medication, for persistent insomnia. JAMA. 2009;301:2005–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. der Lek RF R-v, Swaab DF, Twisk J, Hol EM, Hoogendijk WJG, Someren EJWV. Effect of bright light and melatonin on cognitive and noncognitive function in elderly residents of group care facilities. JAMA. 2008;299:2642–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Van Someren EJW, Lijzenga C, Mirmiran M, Swaab DF. Long-term fitness training improves the circadian rest-activity rhythm in healthy elderly males. J Biol Rhythms. 1997;12:146–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Van Someren EJW. Riemersma-van der Lek RF. Live to the rhythm, slave to the rhythm. Sleep Med. 2007;11:465–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Swaab DF, Van Someren EJW, Zhou JN, Hofman MA. Chapter 23 Biological rhythms in the human life cycle and their relationship to functional changes in the suprachiasmatic nucleus. In: RM Buijs, Kalsbeek A, Romijn HJ, Pennartz CMA, Mirmiran M, editors. Progress in Brain Research. Elsevier; 1996. p. 349–68.

  29. Morin CM, Bootzin RR, Buysse DJ, Edinger JD, Espie CA, Lichstein KL. Psychological And Behavioral Treatment Of Insomnia: Update Of The Recent Evidence (1998–2004). Sleep. 2006;29:1398–414.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Altena E, Van Der Werf YD, Strijers RLM, Van Someren EJW. Sleep loss affects vigilance: effects of chronic insomnia and sleep therapy. J Sleep Res. 2008;17:335–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Gradisar M, Dohnt H, Gardner G, Paine S, Starkey K, Menne A, et al. A Randomized Controlled Trial of Cognitive-Behavior Therapy Plus Bright Light Therapy for Adolescent Delayed Sleep Phase Disorder. Sleep. 2011;34:1671–80.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Riemann D, Perlis ML. The treatments of chronic insomnia: A review of benzodiazepine receptor agonists and psychological and behavioral therapies. Sleep Med Rev. 2009;13:205–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Ritterband LM, Thorndike FP, Gonder-Frederick LA, Magee JC, Bailey ET, Saylor DK, et al. Efficacy of an Internet-Based Behavioral Intervention for Adults With Insomnia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:692–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Vincent N, Lewycky S. Logging on for better sleep: RCT of the effectiveness of online treatment for insomnia. Sleep. 2009;32:807–15.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Czeisler CA, Kronauer RE, Allan JS, Duffy JF, Jewett ME, Brown EN, et al. Bright Light Induction of Strong (Type 0) Resetting of the Human Circadian Pacemaker. Science. 1989;244:1328–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Minors DS, Waterhouse JM, Wirz-Justice A. A human phase-response curve to light. Neurosci Lett. 1991;133:36–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Khalsa SBS, Jewett ME, Cajochen C, Czeisler CA. A phase response curve to single bright light pulses in human subject. J Physiol. 2003;549:945–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Revell VL, Molina TA, Eastman CI. Human phase response curve to intermittent blue light using a commercially available device. J Physiol. 2012;590:4859–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Kobayashi R, Kohsaka M, Fukuda N, Sakakibara S, Honma H, Koyama T. Effects of morning bright light in healthy elderly women: effects on wrist activity. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;53:235–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Van Someren EJW. More than a marker: interaction between the circadian regulation of temperature and sleep, age-related changes, and treatment possibilities. Chronobiol Int. 2000;17:313–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Horne JA, Reid AJ. Night-time sleep EEG changes following body heating in a warm bath. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1985;60:154–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Dorsey CM, Teicher MH, Cohen-Zion M, Stefanovic L, Satlin A, Tartarini W, et al. Core body temperature and sleep of older female insomniacs before and after passive body heating. Sleep. 1999;22:891–8.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Benloucif S, Orbeta L, Ortiz R, Janssen I, Finkel SI, Bleiberg J, et al. Morning or Evening Activity Improves Neuropsychological Performance and Subjective Sleep Quality in Older Adults. Sleep. 2004;27:1542–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Buman MP, Bliwise DL, King AC. Moderators and Mediators of Exercise-Induced Objective Sleep Improvements in Midlife and Older Adults With Sleep Complaints. Health Psychol. 2011;30:579–87.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Buman MP, Hekler EB, Bliwise DL, King AC. Exercise effects on night-to-night fluctuations in self-rated sleep among older adults with sleep complaints. J Sleep Res. 2011;20:28–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Driver HS, Taylor SR. Exercise and sleep. Sleep Med Rev. 2000;4:387–402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Montgomery P, Dennis JA. Physical exercise for sleep problems in adults aged 60+. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2002;4:CD003404.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Passos GS, Poyares D, Santana MG, Garbuio SA, Tufik S, Túlio MM. Effect of Acute Physical Exercise on Patients with Chronic Primary Insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med. 2010;6:270–5.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Passos GS, Poyares D, Santana MG, Rodrigues D’Aurea CV, Youngstedt SD, Tufik S, et al. Effect of moderate aerobic exercise training on chronic primary insomnia. Sleep Med. 2011;12:1018–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Passos GS, Poyares D, Santana MG, Tufik S, Túlio MM. Is exercise an alternative treatment for chronic insomnia? Clinics. 2012;67:653–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Tanaka H, Taira K, Arakawa M, Urasaki C, Yamamoto Y, et al. Short naps and exercise improve sleep quality and mental health in the elderly. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2002;56:233–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Altena E, Van Der Werf YD, Sanz-Arigita EJ, Voorn TA, Rombouts SA, Kuijer JP, et al. Prefrontal hypoactivation and recovery in insomnia. Sleep. 2008;31:1271–6.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Ho FY-Y, Chung K-F, Yeung W-F, Ng TH, Kwan K-S, Yung K-P, et al. Self-help cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Sleep Med Rev. 2015;19:17–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Altena E, Vrenken H, Van Der Werf YD, van den Heuvel OA, Van Someren EJW. Reduced orbitofrontal and parietal gray matter in chronic insomnia: a voxel-based morphometric study. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:182–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Diaz BA, Van Der Sluis S, Moens S, Benjamins JS, Migliorati F, Stoffers D, et al. The Amsterdam resting-state questionnaire reveals multiple phenotypes of resting-state cognition. Front Human Neurosci. 2013;7:446.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Stoffers D, Moens S, Benjamins J, van Tol M-J, Penninx BWHJ, Veltman DJ, et al. Orbitofrontal gray matter relates to early morning awakening: a neural correlate of insomnia complaints? Front Neurol. 2012;3:105. doi:10.3389/fneur.2012.00105.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Zhang B, Wing YK. Sex differences in insomnia: a meta-analysis. Sleep. 2006;29:85–93.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Netherlands Trial Register http://www.trialregister.nl/. Accessed 27 May 2013.

  59. Carney CE, Edinger JD, Olsen MK, Stechuchak KM, Krystal AD, Wyatt JK. Inter-rater reliability for insomnia diagnoses derived from the duke structured interview for sleep disorders. Sleep. 2008;31(Suppl A):250.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Baecke JA, Burema J, Frijters JE. A short questionnaire for the measurement of habitual physical activity in epidemiological studies. Am J Clin Nutr. 1982;36:936–42.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Kang M, Ragan BG, Park J-H. Issues in Outcomes Research: An Overview of Randomization Techniques for Clinical Trials. J Athl Train. 2008;43:215–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Frane JW. A method of biased coin randomization, its implementation, and its validation. Drug Inf J. 1998;32:423–32.

    Google Scholar 

  63. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Sleep Registry. http://www.sleepregistry.org/ Accessed 20 May 2015.

  65. Somnio. http://www.somnio.eu/ Accessed 20 May 2015.

  66. Hofman WF, Kumar A. Online treatment of insomnia using behavioral techniques. Sleep Diagn Ther. 2010;5:28–32.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Carney CE, Buysse DJ, Ancoli-Israel S, Edinger JD, Krystal AD, Lichstein KL, et al. The Consensus Sleep Diary: Standardizing Prospective Sleep Self-Monitoring. Sleep. 2012;35:287–302.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Morin CM, Vallières A, Ivers H. Dysfunctional Beliefs and Attitudes about Sleep (DBAS): Validation of a Brief Version (DBAS-16). Sleep. 2007;30:1547–54.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Eastman CI, Young MA, Fogg LF, Lui L, Meaden PM. Bright Light Treatment of Winter Depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:883–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Goel N, Terman M, Terman JS, Macchi MM, Stewart JW. Controlled trial of bright light and negative air ions for chronic depression. Psychol Med. 2005;35:945–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Terman M, Terman JS. Controlled Trial of Naturalistic Dawn Simulation and Negative Air Ionization for Seasonal Affective Disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:2126–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Terman M, Terman JS, Ross DC. A Controlled Trial of Timed Bright Light and Negative Air Ionization for Treatment of Winter Depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:875–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Andrade AC, Fernandes C, Verghese L, Andrade C. Effect of negative ion atmospheric loading on cognitive performance in human volunteers. Indian J Psychiatry. 1992;34:253–9.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Flory R, Ametepe J, Bowers B. A randomized, placebo-controlled trial of bright light and high density negative air ions for treatment of Seasonal Affective Disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2010;177:101–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Hakkaart-van Roijen L, van Straten A, Donker M, Tiemens B. Handleiding Trimbos/iMTA questionnaire for Costs associated with Psychiatric illness (TiC-P). Erasmus Universiteit Rotterdam: Instituut voor Medische Technology Assessment; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Ware JJ, Sherbourne C. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36). I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Med Care. 1992;30:473–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983;67:361–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54:1063–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Gard DE, Gard MG, Kring AM, John OP. Anticipatory and consummatory components of the experience of pleasure: A scale development study. J Res Person. 2006;40:1086–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Coren S. Prediction of insomnia from arousability predisposition scores: scale development and cross-validation. Behav Res Ther. 1988;26:415–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Pavlova M, Berg O, Gleason R, Walker F, Roberts S, Regestein Q. Self-reported hyperarousal traits among insomnia patients. J Psychosom Res. 2001;51:435–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone S, Hiripi E, et al. The World Health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med. 2005;35:245–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Nicassio PM, Mendlowitz DR, Fussell JJ, Petras L. The phenomenology of the pre-sleep state: The development of the pre-sleep arousal scale. Behav Res Ther. 1985;23:263–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Bastien CH, Vallières A, Morin CM. Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med. 2001;2:297–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Broomfield NM, Espie CA. Towards a valid, reliable measure of sleep effort. J Sleep Res. 2005;14:401–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Lacks P. Behavioral treatment for persistent insomnia. New York: Pergamon Press; 1987.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Vincent N, Sande G, Read C, Giannuzzi T. Sleep Locus of Control: Report on a New Scale. Behav Sleep Med. 2004;2:79–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Vallieres A, Ivers H, Bastien CH, Beaulieu-Bonneau S, Morin CM. Variability and predictability in sleep patterns of chronic insomniacs. J Sleep Res. 2005;14:447–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Van Someren EJW. Improving actigraphic sleep estimates in insomnia and dementia: how many nights? J Sleep Res. 2007;16:269–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Twisk JWR. Applied longitudinal data analysis for epidemiology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  91. Lek R, Swaab DF, Twisk J, Hol EM, Hoogendijk WJG, Van Someren EJW. Effect of bright light and melatonin on cognitive and non-cognitive function in elderly residents of group care facilities: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2008;299:2642–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Morin CM, Belleville G, Belanger L, Ivers H. The insomnia severity index: psychometric indicators to detect insomnia cases and evaluate treatment response. Sleep. 2011;34:601–8.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  93. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic; 1969.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research is supported by Perspective Programme OnTime, project 12188, of the Dutch Technology Foundation STW, which is the applied science division of NWO, and the Technology Programme of the Ministry of Economic Affairs.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kim Dekker.

Additional information

Competing interests

WFH developed the Somnio CBT-I, which is owned and exploited by Personal Health Institute International. The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

KD participates in the study design, patient recruitment and trial coordination, and drafted the manuscript. JSB participates in the study design and trial coordination. AS is the external supervisor and participates in the study design. WFH is the external partner and participates in CBT-I development. EJWS is the principle investigator and initiator of the study, obtained funding, designed the study and supervised writing the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dekker, K., Benjamins, J.S., Van Straten, A. et al. Effectiveness of internet-supported cognitive behavioral and chronobiological interventions and effect moderation by insomnia subtype: study protocol of a randomized controlled trial. Trials 16, 292 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-015-0790-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-015-0790-2

Keywords